When I started this blog, I was a little worried that I
might soon run out of topics. So far, however, the topics have been presenting
themselves. And now readers have even started to suggest topics for blog posts!
I recently received an email message from Etienne LeBel who
said he’d enjoyed my Lazy
Susan and Bruce Springsteen post (Lazy Susan, the gift that keeps on
giving) and suggested I write a post about a
recent positive replication experience of his. Specifically, he said: We really want to get the message out that
these replication efforts need not be adversarial and antagonistic, and that it
should be considered as a normal part of ensuring our science is
self-correcting.
It thought this was a great idea—who wouldn’t want to be the
bearer of good news? So here goes.
The study Lebel and his co-author Lorne Campbell (a great
name for a sheriff in a Western) set out to replicate study 1 of a paper
by Matthew Vess published in Psychological
Science. Vess compared individuals who were more or less “anxiously
attached.” (Being a non-specialist in this area, my first association with the
phrase anxiously attached was that of
a severed limb having been put back in place by a nervous surgeon, but I think
I have a global idea of what it means now.) Vess asked both groups of subjects
about their food preferences. The more anxiously attached subjects reported
heightened preferences for warm foods compared to the more securely attached
subjects. But this occurred only when attachment concerns were activated (i.e.,
reflecting on a romantic breakup) and not in a control condition.
LeBel and Campbell (L&C) say that they are sympathetic
to the theoretical integration of the study but that they wanted to assess the
reproducibility of that finding, given that it was only reported in one study
with 56 subjects.
How did they go about it?
(1) They made sure they had sufficient power to detect the
effect. They didn’t take half-measures and quadrupled the original sample size
so that they had a power of .95 to detect an effect. I recently said on Twitter
that an underpowered (and failed) replication attempt is more like libel than
like research, which seemed to resonate with several people interested in
replication. Clearly, L&C are not guilty of libel.
(2) They contacted the original author, Matthew Vess, for
details about the procedure and materials.
(3) They preregistered the studies prior to data collection.
(4) As in any good replication study, they faithfully copied
the procedure and nature of the sample of the original study.
(5) They went the extra mile by asking the original author to
critique their first attempt. Vess noticed some small discrepancies between
L&C’s first attempt and his original experiment. These discrepancies were
resolved before the second attempt.
(6) They used the exact same analytical procedures as were
used in the original study.
(7) They report their findings in a concise and respectful
manner.
So what did L&C find? Well, they did not replicate the
original finding in either sample. We all can check this because they made all project
materials, raw data, and syntax files available online.
L&C argue that their findings are difficult to reconcile
with the original ones. There were no major procedural discrepancies between
their replication attempts and the original study and they had sufficient power
to detect an effect. And because the replication attempts were preregistered,
selective reporting was not an issue.
L&C conclude: Our
findings, however, do not provide empirical support for the notion that
activating the attachment system of more anxious individuals increases
sensitivity to temperature cues, although it is possible that this theoretical
idea reflects a reproducible phenomenon under a different set of operationalizations.
We therefore advise researchers to proceed with caution when exploring links
between anxious attachment and temperature experiences in potentially
relationship threatening contexts.
This is a nicely worded conclusion. It contains no criticism
of the original study, merely suggesting that researchers in this area should
tread lightly, given that the empirical foundation may not be as strong as
formerly believed. As such, this study is a prime example of what I talked
about in my very
first post: replications should be about checking the structural integrity
of the empirical foundations of the field rather than about pointing fingers.
The steps that L&C followed might serve as a blueprint
for other replication studies. Clearly, researchers need to be true to the
original study in terms of design, procedure, exclusion criteria, and data
analysis. Clearly, they need sufficient power to detect an effect of the size
reported in the original paper. Because of publication bias, this means running
considerably more subjects than in the original study. Pre-registration seems
the way to go, as it prevents a reverse file-drawer problem in which people
will only report non-replications, as these might seem more informative than
replications.
It is also important to consult the original author. Obviously, original authors may not always be as
helpful as Vess evidently was in this case but they should at least be consulted
regarding the materials, design, procedure, and analyses of the intended replication
attempt.
L&C’s paper is currently in press in Psychological Science, the journal that
also published the original study by Vess.
The original findings may not have been replicated, but I’d still call this a successful replication attempt.
The original findings may not have been replicated, but I’d still call this a successful replication attempt.
Even better, when Eric Eich (Editor of Psych Science offered Vess an opportunity to respond, this is what Vess said:
BeantwoordenVerwijderen"Thank you for the opportunity to submit a rejoinder to LeBel and Campbell’s commentary. I have, however, decided not to submit one. While I am certainly dismayed to see the failed attempts to reproduce a published study of mine, I am in agreement with the journal’s decision to publish the replication studies in a commentary and believe that such decisions will facilitate the advancement of psychological science and the collaborative pursuit of accurate knowledge. LeBel and Campbell provide a fair and reasonable interpretation of what their findings mean for using this paradigm to study attachment and temperature associations, and I appreciated their willingness to consult me in the development of their replication efforts. Once again, thank you for the opportunity."
A model of civilized response. I don't know which is stronger, my admiration for this response or my dismay that this kind of responses is (to date) so rare.
I truly admire the response! I wish we (including me) could all be this civil.
BeantwoordenVerwijderen